The post What Is Ascertainment Bias? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>Ascertainment bias is a form of selection bias and is related to sampling bias. In medical research, the term ascertainment bias is more common than the term sampling bias.
Ascertainment bias is a form of systematic error that occurs during data collection and analysis. It occurs when sample units are drawn in such a way that those selected are not representative of the target population.
In medical research, ascertainment bias also refers to situations where the results of a clinical trial are distorted due to knowledge about which intervention each participant is receiving.
Ascertainment bias can be introduced by:
Ascertainment bias can influence the generalizability of your results and threaten the external validity of your findings.
There are two main sources of ascertainment bias:
Blinding is an important methodological feature of placebo-controlled trials to minimize research bias and maximize the validity of the research results.
In experimental studies, ascertainment bias can be reduced by “blinding” everyone involved, including those who administer the intervention, those who receive it, and those concerned with assessing and reporting the results. This is called triple blinding.
More specifically, ascertainment bias can be avoided in the following ways during the data collection phase:
This also reduces the risk of introducing other types of bias, such as demand characteristics and confirmation bias.
Keep in mind that bias can also be introduced after data collection. To reduce ascertainment bias in this phase, make sure that:
Lastly, ascertainment bias can also affect observational studies because subjects cannot be randomized. In this case, you can reduce ascertainment bias by carefully describing the inclusion and exclusion criteria used for selecting subjects or cases.
Bias affects the validity and reliability of your findings, leading to false conclusions and a misinterpretation of the truth. This can have serious implications in areas like medical research where new forms of treatment are being evaluated.
Common types of selection bias are:
Reliability and validity are both about how well a method measures something:
If you are doing experimental research, you also have to consider the internal and external validity of your experiment.
The post What Is Ascertainment Bias? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post What Is the Placebo Effect? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The placebo effect is often observed in experimental designs where participants are randomly assigned to either a control or treatment group.
A placebo can be a sugar pill, a salt water injection, or even a fake surgical procedure. In other words, a placebo has no therapeutic properties. Placebos are often used in medical research and clinical trials to help scientists evaluate the effects of new medications.
In these clinical trials, participants are randomly assigned to either the placebo or the experimental medication. Crucially, they are not aware of which treatment they receive. The results of the two groups are compared, to see whether they differ.
In double-blind studies, researchers also don’t know who received the actual treatment or the placebo. This is to prevent them from conveying demand characteristics to participants that could influence the study’s results. This is preferred over single-blind studies, where participants do not know which group they have been placed in, but researchers do.
Placebos may help relieve symptoms like pain, fatigue, or stress-related insomnia, but they don’t actually treat a condition or cure a disease. Note that due to ethical considerations, placebos are not always used in clinical trials. For example, as it would be unethical to leave terminal cancer patients untreated, placebos aren’t used in these types of studies.
For some people, just the idea that they are taking medication makes them feel better. This occurs even if the medication is actually just a placebo. This phenomenon is known as the placebo effect. In other words, the perception of feeling better is triggered by the person’s belief in the benefit of the treatment.
When studying a new treatment, researchers must demonstrate that it is more effective than can just be explained by the placebo effect. To do so, they compare the results from those taking the new treatment are with those from the placebo. In order to accurately compare the two groups, participants in clinical trials must not know whether they received the treatment or the placebo. If the two groups have the same reaction, the new treatment is deemed ineffective.
When evaluating the effectiveness of the new treatment, researchers then exclude the placebo effect. Studies on the placebo effect indicate that 21% to 40% of study participants taking placebos will experience relief from symptoms.
Although the exact reasons for the positive effects of placebos are still being researched, a number of factors contribute to the phenomenon. These include:
However, researchers do not attribute the placebo effect exclusively to psychology. A few other possible explanations include:
The placebo effect illustrates how the mind can trigger changes in the body.
The placebo effect can also explain the popularity of non-FDA-approved products.
The response of people assigned to the placebo control group may not always be positive. They may experience what is called a “nocebo effect,” or a negative outcome, when taking a placebo. The same explanation applies here. If you expect a negative outcome, it’s more likely you’ll have a negative outcome.
For example, in a clinical trial, participants who are given a placebo but are told what side effects the “treatment” may cause. They may have the same side effects as the participants who are given the active treatment, only because they expect them to occur.
Although there is no definite answer to what causes the placebo effect, researchers propose a number of explanations such as the power of suggestion, doctor-patient interaction, classical conditioning, etc.
Placebos are used in medical research for new medication or therapies, called clinical trials. In these trials some people are given a placebo, while others are given the new medication being tested.
The purpose is to determine how effective the new medication is: if it benefits people beyond a predefined threshold as compared to the placebo, it’s considered effective.
Bias affects the validity and reliability of your findings, leading to false conclusions and a misinterpretation of the truth. This can have serious implications in areas like medical research where new forms of treatment are being evaluated.
The post What Is the Placebo Effect? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post Regression to the Mean | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>Regression to the mean is due to natural variation or chance. It can be observed in everyday life, particularly in research that intentionally focuses on the most extreme cases or events. It is sometimes also called regression toward the mean.
Regression to the mean is common in repeated measurements (within-subject designs) and should always be considered as a possible cause of an observed change. It is considered a type of information bias and can distort research findings.
Regression to the mean is observed when variables that are extremely higher or extremely lower than average on the first measurement move closer to the average on the second measurement.
In general, RTM explains why unusual events are likely to be followed by more typical ones. Suppose that a company has a great quarter, exceeding all targets set. As exceptional performance is difficult to maintain over time, there is a high chance of worse performance in the next quarter, shifting the performance of the company back towards the mean. Anything that can be influenced by an element of chance is subject to this phenomenon.
Regression to the mean occurs when a nonrandom sample is selected from a population and you measure two imperfectly correlated variables, such as two consecutive blood pressure measurements.
Regression to the mean can be explained by considering, for example, that skill and performance are imperfectly correlated due to the role of luck. This may lead you to find a causal relationship where there isn’t one.
Regression to the mean can prove problematic particularly in research studies that measure the effectiveness of an intervention, program, or policy.
It can mislead researchers to believe that an intervention is the cause of an observed change, when in reality it is due to chance. This is particularly evident when researchers focus on measurements of people, cases, or organizations at the extremes, such as the worst-performing, the best-educated, or the unhealthiest.
RTM shows us that, statistically, the lowest cases are likely to improve the second time, while those at their peak will likely perform worse even without the intervention. Because it can distort results, you need to take regression to the mean into account when designing your research as well as when analyzing your findings.
Otherwise, you run the risk of attributing certain results to a particular cause, when in reality they were most likely due to chance.
Regression to the mean often happens when measuring the effects of an intervention.
Relatedly, randomized evaluations are essential in avoiding regression to the mean when estimating the effects of an intervention.
The best way to avoid regression to the mean is to account for it during the design phase of your research. Whenever possible, use a probability sampling method. Otherwise, your results may lean towards the extremes, either abnormally high or abnormally low for the average. These will, by design, regress towards the mean upon retesting.
In experimental designs, it is important to use a control group and a treatment group, randomly assigning participants to each one. Changes in the control group can help you evaluate the extent of changes caused by regression to the mean, in addition to any placebo effect. Any extra improvement or deterioration in the experimental group compared to the control group can be attributed to the intervention, so long as it is statistically significant.
Alternatively, you can calculate the percent of regression to the mean during your data analysis. You can use the formula below to calculate regression to the mean.
Information bias is a general term describing various forms of research bias arising due to systematic measurement error. The main types of information bias are:
A correlation coefficient is a single number that describes the strength and direction of the relationship between your variables.
Different types of correlation coefficients might be appropriate for your data based on their levels of measurement and distributions. The Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (Pearson’s r) is commonly used to assess a linear relationship between two quantitative variables.
The third variable and directionality problems are two main reasons why correlation isn’t causation.
The third variable problem means that a confounding variable affects both variables to make them seem causally related when they are not.
The directionality problem is when two variables correlate and might actually have a causal relationship, but it’s impossible to conclude which variable causes changes in the other.
The post Regression to the Mean | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post Personal Pronouns | Definition, List, & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>Like other pronouns, they are used in place of nouns to allow us to speak and write more concisely. For example, without the first-person pronoun “I,” you would have to use your name every time you wanted to make a statement about yourself.
Personal pronouns change form based on person, number, gender, and case, as shown in the table below.
Person | Number (& gender) | Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
First | Singular | I | me | mine | myself |
Plural | we | us | ours | ourselves | |
Second | Singular | you | yours | yourself | |
Plural | you | yours | yourselves | ||
Third |
Masculine singular | he | him | his | himself |
Feminine singular | she | her | hers | herself | |
Neuter / inanimate singular | it | its | itself | ||
Gender-neutral singular (epicene) | they | them | theirs | themself | |
Plural | they | them | theirs | themselves | |
Impersonal | one | — | oneself |
Four factors indicate which personal pronoun you should use in a particular grammatical context to refer to a particular person or thing. These are:
Personal pronouns are called “personal” not because they always refer to people (“it,” for example, doesn’t) but because they indicate a grammatical feature called person. There are three possibilities:
Number indicates whether the personal pronoun refers to an individual person or thing or to a group of two or more.
Gender is how personal pronouns indicate the gender of the person referred to—or the lack of gender of objects and concepts.
Case means the grammatical role that the pronoun plays in a sentence. Personal pronouns can play four different roles in a sentence:
The first-person pronouns don’t vary based on gender, but they do vary based on number and case, as shown in the table below. The first-person singular subject pronoun “I” is the only English pronoun that is always capitalized.
Note that there is ongoing debate about the use of first-person pronouns in academic writing.
Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | I | me | mine | myself |
Plural | we | us | ours | ourselves |
The second-person pronouns also do not vary based on gender, only the reflexive form varies based on number, and the subject and object forms are the same. This makes them the least variable set of personal pronouns but can occasionally lead to ambiguity.
Second-person pronouns should almost never be used in academic writing, as addressing the reader directly is seen as too informal.
Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular | you | yours | yourself | |
Plural | you | yours | yourselves |
The third-person pronouns are much more variable than the first- and second-person pronouns, since they also change form based on gender, in addition to number and case.
As well as forms for the masculine and feminine, there is a neuter (or inanimate) form that’s used to refer to things other than people (e.g., ideas, objects, animals).
There’s also an increasingly widely used gender-neutral (or epicene) form, the singular “they.” This is largely identical to the plural form (which is always gender-neutral), except that the reflexive “themself” is sometimes used instead of “themselves” (though it’s often considered nonstandard).
Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Masculine singular | he | him | his | himself |
Feminine singular | she | her | hers | herself |
Neuter / inanimate singular | it | its | itself | |
Gender-neutral singular (epicene) | they | them | theirs | themself |
Plural | they | them | theirs | themselves |
The impersonal pronoun “one,” as the name suggests, doesn’t vary based on person—it’s not in the first, second, or third person.
Rather, like an indefinite pronoun, it refers to a nonspecific, generic individual, usually for the purpose of making a generalization or stating a principle. It’s considered quite formal and often replaced with “you,” or otherwise avoided, in informal contexts.
The impersonal pronoun doesn’t vary based on number or gender, and it has the same form whether used as a subject or object. It does have a separate form for the reflexive, but no possessive pronoun form.
Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive |
---|---|---|---|
one | — | oneself |
There are a few other personal pronouns that are rarely used, nonstandard, or archaic (no longer used). These generally shouldn’t show up in your academic or formal writing, but it’s worth knowing they exist.
The lack of variety in English second-person pronouns is somewhat unusual, as other languages (e.g., French) make clearer distinctions between singular and plural and between formal and informal ways of addressing someone.
Early Modern English used a larger set of second-person pronouns to convey this kind of distinction. The “th” pronouns were used for informal address, while the “y” pronouns were used for both formal address and plurals.
These additional pronouns are not used in contemporary standard English unless a deliberate attempt is being made to imitate old-fashioned or biblical language in a humorous or literary context. Some of them have survived in certain dialects of English.
Subject | Object | Possessive | Reflexive | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Singular informal | thou | thee | thine | thyself |
Singular formal | ye / you | you | yours | yourself |
Plural | ye / you | you | yours | yourself |
The lack of distinction between singular and plural in the standard second-person pronouns has given rise to various ways of expressing the plural in different dialects.
Though some of these are very widely used in everyday speech, they are all still regarded as nonstandard and not used in formal or academic writing. Some examples are given in the table below.
Pronoun | Notes |
---|---|
y’all | Used in the US, especially in the South and in AAVE |
yinz | Used mainly in Pittsburgh, PA, and the surrounding area |
you guys | Used in the US and increasingly in Canada, the UK, and Australia; mostly used in a gender-neutral sense despite containing the word “guys” |
you lot | Used in the UK and Australia |
yous(e) | Used in Ireland and various regions of the UK, as well as other parts of the world such as Australia and parts of Canada |
In some contexts, an individual might refer to themselves as “we” and therefore use the alternative reflexive pronoun ourself.
This commonly occurs with the royal we (used by monarchs), the editorial we (used by an individual speaking for a publication or organization), and the generic we (used to make generalizations). It’s not advisable to use any of these, or the word “ourself,” in academic writing.
Other nonstandard reflexive pronouns are hisself (replacing “himself”), theirselves (replacing “themselves”), and theirself (replacing “themself”). These are all widely regarded as mistakes and should be avoided in writing generally, whether formal or informal.
The third-person plural object pronoun “them” is often replaced by ’em in informal contexts. It’s older than you’d expect, believed to actually be an abbreviation of the Middle English pronoun “hem” rather than the current pronoun “them.” But it’s not used in formal or academic writing.
The second- and third-person pronouns He/Him/His/Himself, She/Her/Hers/Herself, and You/Yours/Yourself are sometimes capitalized in a religious context when they are used to refer to a deity.
This is commonly encountered in sacred works such as the Bible or the Quran and in the writing of other religious figures, though it’s not always done consistently. It’s not necessary to imitate this usage in a nonreligious context.
Personal pronouns are words like “he,” “me,” and “yourselves” that refer to the person you’re addressing, to other people or things, or to yourself. Like other pronouns, they usually stand in for previously mentioned nouns (antecedents).
They are called “personal” not because they always refer to people (e.g., “it” doesn’t) but because they indicate grammatical person (first, second, or third person). Personal pronouns also change their forms based on number, gender, and grammatical role in a sentence.
In grammar, person is how we distinguish between the speaker or writer (first person), the person being addressed (second person), and any other people, objects, ideas, etc. referred to (third person).
Person is expressed through the different personal pronouns, such as “I” (first-person pronoun), “you” (second-person pronoun), and “they” (third-person pronoun). It also affects how verbs are conjugated, due to subject-verb agreement (e.g., “I am” vs. “you are”).
In fiction, a first-person narrative is one written directly from the perspective of the protagonist. A third-person narrative describes the protagonist from the perspective of a separate narrator. A second-person narrative (very rare) addresses the reader as if they were the protagonist.
The term preferred pronouns is used to mean the personal pronouns a person identifies with and would like to be referred to by. People usually state the subject and object pronoun (e.g., “she/her”) but may also include the possessive (e.g., “she/her/hers”).
Most people go by the masculine “he/him,” the feminine “she/her,” the gender-neutral singular “they/them,” or some combination of these. There are also neopronouns used to express nonbinary gender identity, such as “xe/xem.” These are less common than the singular “they.”
The practice of stating one’s preferred pronouns (e.g., in a professional context or on a social media profile) is meant to promote inclusion for transgender and gender-non-conforming people.
The post Personal Pronouns | Definition, List, & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post How to Write a Research Proposal | Examples & Templates appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>A research proposal describes what you will investigate, why it’s important, and how you will conduct your research.
The format of a research proposal varies between fields, but most proposals will contain at least these elements:
While the sections may vary, the overall objective is always the same. A research proposal serves as a blueprint and guide for your research plan, helping you get organized and feel confident in the path forward you choose to take.
Academics often have to write research proposals to get funding for their projects. As a student, you might have to write a research proposal as part of a grad school application, or prior to starting your thesis or dissertation.
In addition to helping you figure out what your research can look like, a proposal can also serve to demonstrate why your project is worth pursuing to a funder, educational institution, or supervisor.
Relevance | Show your reader why your project is interesting, original, and important. |
Context | Demonstrate your comfort and familiarity with your field. Show that you understand the current state of research on your topic. |
Approach | Make a case for your methodology. Demonstrate that you have carefully thought about the data, tools, and procedures necessary to conduct your research. |
Achievability | Confirm that your project is feasible within the timeline of your program or funding deadline. |
The length of a research proposal can vary quite a bit. A bachelor’s or master’s thesis proposal can be just a few pages, while proposals for PhD dissertations or research funding are usually much longer and more detailed. Your supervisor can help you determine the best length for your work.
One trick to get started is to think of your proposal’s structure as a shorter version of your thesis or dissertation, only without the results, conclusion and discussion sections.
Download our research proposal template
Writing a research proposal can be quite challenging, but a good starting point could be to look at some examples. We’ve included a few for you below.
Like your dissertation or thesis, the proposal will usually have a title page that includes:
The first part of your proposal is the initial pitch for your project. Make sure it succinctly explains what you want to do and why.
Your introduction should:
To guide your introduction, include information about:
As you get started, it’s important to demonstrate that you’re familiar with the most important research on your topic. A strong literature review shows your reader that your project has a solid foundation in existing knowledge or theory. It also shows that you’re not simply repeating what other people have already done or said, but rather using existing research as a jumping-off point for your own.
In this section, share exactly how your project will contribute to ongoing conversations in the field by:
Following the literature review, restate your main objectives. This brings the focus back to your own project. Next, your research design or methodology section will describe your overall approach, and the practical steps you will take to answer your research questions.
Research type |
|
Population and sample |
|
Research methods |
|
Practicalities |
|
To finish your proposal on a strong note, explore the potential implications of your research for your field. Emphasize again what you aim to contribute and why it matters.
For example, your results might have implications for:
Last but not least, your research proposal must include correct citations for every source you have used, compiled in a reference list. To create citations quickly and easily, you can use our free APA citation generator.
Some institutions or funders require a detailed timeline of the project, asking you to forecast what you will do at each stage and how long it may take. While not always required, be sure to check the requirements of your project.
Here’s an example schedule to help you get started. You can also download a template at the button below.
Download our research schedule template
Research phase | Objectives | Deadline |
---|---|---|
1. Background research and literature review |
|
20th January |
2. Research design planning |
|
13th February |
3. Data collection and preparation |
|
24th March |
4. Data analysis |
|
22nd April |
5. Writing |
|
17th June |
6. Revision |
|
28th July |
If you are applying for research funding, chances are you will have to include a detailed budget. This shows your estimates of how much each part of your project will cost.
Make sure to check what type of costs the funding body will agree to cover. For each item, include:
To determine your budget, think about:
Once you’ve decided on your research objectives, you need to explain them in your paper, at the end of your problem statement.
Keep your research objectives clear and concise, and use appropriate verbs to accurately convey the work that you will carry out for each one.
A research aim is a broad statement indicating the general purpose of your research project. It should appear in your introduction at the end of your problem statement, before your research objectives.
Research objectives are more specific than your research aim. They indicate the specific ways you’ll address the overarching aim.
A PhD, which is short for philosophiae doctor (doctor of philosophy in Latin), is the highest university degree that can be obtained. In a PhD, students spend 3–5 years writing a dissertation, which aims to make a significant, original contribution to current knowledge.
A PhD is intended to prepare students for a career as a researcher, whether that be in academia, the public sector, or the private sector.
A master’s is a 1- or 2-year graduate degree that can prepare you for a variety of careers.
All master’s involve graduate-level coursework. Some are research-intensive and intend to prepare students for further study in a PhD; these usually require their students to write a master’s thesis. Others focus on professional training for a specific career.
Critical thinking refers to the ability to evaluate information and to be aware of biases or assumptions, including your own.
Like information literacy, it involves evaluating arguments, identifying and solving problems in an objective and systematic way, and clearly communicating your ideas.
The post How to Write a Research Proposal | Examples & Templates appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post Alike | Definition, Meaning & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>
Look-alike is a noun referring to a person or thing that looks like someone or something else. It can also be used as an adjective when it occurs before a noun. The plural form of look-alike is look-alikes.
Note that only the noun is hyphenated; no hyphen should be added when using “look alike” as a verb: “They’re brothers, but they don’t look alike.”
Great minds think alike is a humorous expression used to acknowledge that two or more people share the same opinion or have reached the same conclusion at the same time. It’s typically used by one of the like-minded people to mean “we are very clever.”
Share and share alike is an idiom used to emphasize that each member of a group should receive an equal share of something. It’s often used in a legal context, such as in a will, to indicate that each individual should receive the same amount.
If you want to know more about commonly confused words, definitions, and differences between US and UK spellings, make sure to check out some of our other language articles with explanations, examples, and quizzes.
Confused words
Definitions
US vs. UK spellings
There are numerous synonyms and near-synonyms for the two meanings of alike.
Similar (adjective) | Similarly (adverb) |
---|---|
Identical | Correspondingly |
Indistinguishable | Identically |
Interchangeable | Likewise |
Matching | The same |
The same | Uniformly |
Undifferentiated | |
Uniform |
The post Alike | Definition, Meaning & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post What Is Generalizability? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>Generalizability is determined by how representative your sample is of the target population. This is known as external validity.
The goal of research is to produce knowledge that can be applied as widely as possible. However, since it usually isn’t possible to analyze every member of a population, researchers make do by analyzing a portion of it, making statements about that portion.
To be able to apply these statements to larger groups, researchers must ensure that the sample accurately resembles the broader population.
In other words, the sample and the population must share the characteristics relevant to the research being conducted. When this happens, the sample is considered representative, and by extension, the study’s results are considered generalizable.
In general, a study has good generalizability when the results apply to many different types of people or different situations. In contrast, if the results can only be applied to a subgroup of the population or in a very specific situation, the study has poor generalizability.
Obtaining a representative sample is crucial for probability sampling. In contrast, studies using non-probability sampling designs are more concerned with investigating a few cases in depth, rather than generalizing their findings. As such, generalizability is the main difference between probability and non-probability samples.
There are three factors that determine the generalizability of your study in a probability sampling design:
Generalizability is one of the three criteria (along with validity and reliability) that researchers use to assess the quality of both quantitative and qualitative research. However, depending on the type of research, generalizability is interpreted and evaluated differently.
Generalizability is crucial for establishing the validity and reliability of your study. In most cases, a lack of generalizability significantly narrows down the scope of your research—i.e., to whom the results can be applied.
However, research results that cannot be generalized can still have value. It all depends on your research objectives.
There are two broad types of generalizability:
Statistical generalizability is critical for quantitative research. The goal of quantitative research is to develop general knowledge that applies to all the units of a population while studying only a subset of these units (sample). Statistical generalization is achieved when you study a sample that accurately mirrors characteristics of the population. The sample needs to be sufficiently large and unbiased.
In qualitative research, statistical generalizability is not relevant. This is because qualitative research is primarily concerned with obtaining insights on some aspect of human experience, rather than data with solid statistical basis. By studying individual cases, researchers will try to get results that they can extend to similar cases. This is known as theoretical generalizability or transferability.
In order to apply your findings on a larger scale, you should take the following steps to ensure your research has sufficient generalizability.
After completing your research, take a moment to reflect on the generalizability of your findings. What didn’t go as planned and could impact your generalizability? For example, selection biases such as non-response bias can affect your results. Explain how generalizable your results are, as well as possible limitations, in the discussion section of your research paper.
Generalizability is important because it allows researchers to make inferences for a large group of people, i.e., the target population, by only studying a part of it (the sample).
Internal validity is the degree of confidence that the causal relationship you are testing is not influenced by other factors or variables.
External validity is the extent to which your results can be generalized to other contexts.
The validity of your experiment depends on your experimental design.
In the discussion, you explore the meaning and relevance of your research results, explaining how they fit with existing research and theory. Discuss:
The post What Is Generalizability? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post Who vs. Whom | Examples, Definition & Quiz appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>Who and whom are used to refer to people and sometimes animals.
Examples: Who in a sentence | Examples: Whom in a sentence |
---|---|
Who is at the door? | To whom should I speak? |
Who knows the answer? | With whom do you want to work? |
Jamil, who just started last week, is already excelling at his new job. | Fia, whom I have known for years, is getting married next week. |
Who is a pronoun that functions as the subject of a sentence, so it will always refer to the person performing the action.
Who can be used as an interrogative pronoun to ask a question.
It can also be used as a relative pronoun (i.e., a pronoun that refers to a previously mentioned noun) to connect a main clause to a relative clause.
When a relative clause is restrictive (i.e., provides essential information about the noun), it is not separated from the main clause. If a relative clause is non-restrictive (i.e., does not provide essential information), it is set off from the main clause with commas.
Whom is a pronoun that acts as the object of a verb or preposition (often the person that is acted upon).
It can also be used as a relative pronoun to connect a relative clause to a main clause.
If a relative clause provides essential information, it is not separated from the main clause. If it does not provide essential information, it is set off from the main clause using commas.
If you aren’t sure whether you’re using who or whom correctly, try determining what type of pronoun is needed.
If you can answer a question using a subject pronoun, or if the sentence can be rearranged using the same verb and a subject pronoun, who is correct.
However, if the sentence requires an object pronoun, you need whom.
To whom it may concern is a phrase used at the start of formal or professional correspondence when you don’t know the name of the person you’re addressing. “To who it may concern” is never correct.
Test your knowledge of the difference between “whom” and “who” by using our practice quiz below. Fill in either “whom” or “who” in each sentence.
If you want to know more about commonly confused words, definitions, and differences between US and UK spellings, make sure to check out some of our other language articles with explanations, examples, and quizzes.
Confused words
Definitions
US vs. UK spellings
The post Who vs. Whom | Examples, Definition & Quiz appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post 10 Best Free Grammar Checkers | Tested & Reviewed appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>To find out, we tested 10 of the most popular free grammar checkers, checking how many errors they could fix in our sample text and deducting points for any new errors introduced. We also assessed the tools’ user-friendliness. The results show a clear winner: QuillBot.
Grammar checker | Corrections score | Overall rating |
---|---|---|
1. QuillBot | 18 out of 20 | 4.8 |
2. GrammarCheck | 11 out of 20 | 2.9 |
3. Grammarly | 11 out of 20 | 2.9 |
4. Wordtune | 13 out of 20 | 2.6 |
5. LanguageTool | 6 out of 20 | 2.5 |
6. Writer | 3 out of 20 | 1.9 |
7. ProWritingAid | 3 out of 20 | 1.6 |
8. Linguix | 3 out of 20 | 1.3 |
9. Scribens | 3 out of 20 | 1.1 |
10. Ginger | 2 out of 20 | 0.9 |
QuillBot performed exceptionally well compared to the other tools we tested for this comparison. It was able to find and correct 19 of the 20 errors.
It did appear to notice the 20th error, but its attempt to fix it was not effective and introduced a new issue, so we deducted a point, resulting in a score of 18 out of 20. Despite this bug, that’s drastically higher than the second-highest scorer, Wordtune (13 out of 20).
We also found the QuillBot website to be very user-friendly. The “Fix All Errors” option saves a lot of time and clicking when a high number of corrections are needed. And getting started is very quick, since no sign-up is required and you can just copy text directly into the site.
Check out QuillBot’s grammar checker
Though it performed a lot worse than QuillBot, GrammarCheck did better than the average free grammar checker. It corrected 11 out of 20 errors in the text and did not introduce any errors in the process.
In terms of usability, we found GrammarCheck straightforward to use, since there’s no sign-up required and you can just copy-paste text into the site. It also takes only one click to accept a change in the text.
But the interface on the site looks quite dated, and convenient options like a “Fix All Errors” button are missing. The “Deep Check” button seems to redirect the user to Grammarly, although GrammarCheck’s own tool does seem slightly different from Grammarly’s.
Grammarly performs well compared to the average grammar checker. But because it’s not entirely free, many of the issues it notices can only be checked if you pay for a premium membership. The free version of Grammarly could only fix 11 out of 20 errors in our text.
Based on the other parts of the text it had highlighted, it looks like premium version of Grammarly would be able to detect 15 of the 20 errors—notably, still a lower number than QuillBot detects and fixes for free.
In terms of usability, we found it somewhat inconvenient to have to sign up to use the grammar checker, but we appreciated the clean look of the interface and the fact that it gave multiple options for corrections when different interpretations were possible. However, we found the process of fixing errors slower than it needed to be.
Wordtune performed better than any other grammar checker except QuillBot in terms of the errors it found and corrected, with a score of 13 out of 20. However, this is with the important caveat that it’s more of a paraphrasing tool than a standard grammar checker.
As far as we could tell, there was no way to separate its paraphrasing functionality from its basic grammar checking, so the only way it would fix most errors in the text was by completely rewriting sentences. This makes it an inappropriate choice if you want to keep the structure of your text intact.
We also found that the paraphrases offered would sometimes introduce new errors or significantly change your meaning. And we found it inconvenient to have to sign up before using the tool. For these reasons, we’ve ranked it lower than some other tools despite its higher score “on paper.”
LanguageTool performed relatively poorly, detecting only 6 out of 20 errors in the text. For the most part, it was only able to recognize spelling mistakes and word choice errors, though it did notice one punctuation error.
It also labeled the word “apraxia” a “possible spelling mistake.” Since it gave the option of adding the word to a personal dictionary, we haven’t deducted a point for this. But we do think failure to recognize a word that appears in many widely used dictionaries is a bad sign.
We did find the site relatively user-friendly, since it doesn’t require a sign-up and has a decent user interface. But this isn’t enough to make up for its poor level of error detection.
Writer corrected 7 errors in the text, a slightly better performance than LanguageTool, but it also introduced 4 new errors in the process of correcting them, resulting in a score of 3 out of 20.
The errors it introduced were all punctuation errors. Writer seems to attempt to simplify your sentence structure in a clumsy way, by simply removing commas and parentheses that are grammatically necessary. This results in confusing and grammatically incorrect sentences.
The interface on the Writer site is fairly clean and usable, and it does allow you to accept each change with one click. But you have to go through a sign-up process before you can start using it. And due to the various errors introduced, it’s hard to recommend Writer.
Like Writer, ProWritingAid was able to find only a few errors and introduced some errors of its own. In this case, it fixed 5 errors and introduced 2, resulting in a score of 3 out of 20.
The errors introduced by ProWritingAid resulted from identifying issues but fixing them in ways that didn’t work. For example, it identified that the acronym “C.O.P.D” is not correct, but suggested the solution “C.O.P. D” with a space inserted before the “D”—which is just as bad.
We found the interface only moderately user-friendly. The interface of the tool looks fairly appealing, but it’s also very busy, with a lot of unnecessary information and options displayed in a distracting way. It also can’t be used without first signing up for an account.
Linguix had the worst performance out of any of the tools we tested, fixing only 3 errors and introducing 1 new error, resulting in a score of 2 out of 20. It highlighted 7 other errors, but as with some other tools, it would only explain and correct them in the premium version.
Like LanguageTool, it was unable to recognize the word “apraxia.” In this case, we’ve deducted a point for this, since it simply labels the word as a “spelling mistake” without any suggestion it could be misleading the user.
It also wasn’t all that user-friendly. While the interface looks nice enough and one-click corrections are possible, you have to sign up to edit texts above a certain length. And since even the premium version seems to find less than half the errors in the text, we can’t recommend it.
Scribens (not to be confused with Scribbr!) scored the same as ProWritingAid, with 5 errors fixed and 2 new ones introduced, resulting in a score of 3 out of 20. In one case, it suggested changing “multiple sclerosis” to “many sclerosis,” quite a bad mistake.
Scribens also offers a variety of extra features that can be turned on at the side. We found that these features introduced various major errors into the text when turned on, such as changing “they’re” into “they’represent.” The one that claimed to detect run-on sentences actually appeared to just highlight every long sentence.
Since these features were turned off by default, we haven’t deducted points for them, but we strongly recommend against using them. In terms of user interface, Scribens is at least easy to get started with, but it’s quite dated in appearance and performs very poorly overall.
Ginger scored the same as several other tools we tested, finding only 3 out of 20 errors but not adding any errors of its own. We’ve ranked it lowest out of all the tools we tested because of some unreasonable limitations regarding text length.
Ginger claims that its grammar checker will only “partially” correct sentences of more than 350 characters. In practice, we found that it just ignored these sentences entirely, making it useless for checking any kind of academic text, where long sentences are common.
It also wouldn’t accept texts longer than 900 characters (450 if you don’t sign up), meaning we had to check the two paragraphs of our text separately. On the whole, we found the Ginger tool to be exceptionally poor and impractical.
To compare the capabilities of different grammar checkers objectively, we used the same text for all tests and applied the same criteria to our assessment of the results.
To test each grammar checker, we used the same 160-word text, shown below. It contains 20 grammatical, spelling, word choice, or punctuation errors. You can see a description of each error by mousing over them in the text.
We pasted the text above into each of the grammar checkers and assessed how effectively they were able to correct the errors. Each checker received a score out of 20 based on the following rules:
For example, if the tool “fixed” an existing error but introduced a new error in the process, it would gain no points for that error. If it introduced an error without fixing the existing one, it would lose a point.
The usability of each tool was assessed qualitatively based on factors like how quickly the errors could be corrected, whether a sign-up was required, how clear and user-friendly the interface was, and whether the tool could check the entire text at once.
Our research indicates that the best free grammar checker available online is the QuillBot grammar checker.
We tested 10 of the most popular checkers with the same sample text (containing 20 grammatical errors) and found that QuillBot easily outperformed the competition, scoring 18 out of 20, a drastic improvement over the second-place score of 13 out of 20.
It even appeared to outperform the premium versions of other grammar checkers, despite being entirely free.
Good grammar is the key to expressing yourself clearly and fluently, especially in professional communication and academic writing. Word processors, browsers, and email programs typically have built-in grammar checkers, but they’re quite limited in the kinds of problems they can fix.
If you want to go beyond detecting basic spelling errors, there are many online grammar checkers with more advanced functionality. They can often detect issues with punctuation, word choice, and sentence structure that more basic tools would miss.
Not all of these tools are reliable, though. You can check out our research into the best free grammar checkers to explore the options.
A grammar checker is a tool designed to automatically check your text for spelling errors, grammatical issues, punctuation mistakes, and problems with sentence structure. You can check out our analysis of the best free grammar checkers to learn more.
A paraphrasing tool edits your text more actively, changing things whether they were grammatically incorrect or not. It can paraphrase your sentences to make them more concise and readable or for other purposes. You can check out our analysis of the best free paraphrasing tools to learn more.
Some tools available online combine both functions. Others, such as QuillBot, have separate grammar checker and paraphrasing tools. Be aware of what exactly the tool you’re using does to avoid introducing unwanted changes.
The post 10 Best Free Grammar Checkers | Tested & Reviewed appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>The post What Is Survivorship Bias? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>
Survivorship bias is a form of selection bias. It occurs when a dataset only considers existing (or “surviving”) observations and fails to consider observations that have ceased to exist.
For example, when investigating the profitability of the tech industry, one has to also study businesses that went bankrupt, rather than only focusing on businesses currently in the market.
Focusing on a subset of your sample that has already passed some sort of selection process increases your chances of drawing incorrect conclusions. “Surviving” observations may have survived exactly because they are more resilient to difficult conditions, while others have ceased to exist as a result of those same conditions.
When a study is affected by survivorship bias, we only pay attention to part of the data. This can have a number of consequences, such as:
Awareness of survivorship bias is important because it impacts our perception, judgment, and the quality of our conclusions.
Survivorship bias can cloud our judgment not only in research, but in everyday life, too.
Relatedly, before drawing any conclusions, you need to ask yourself whether your dataset is truly complete. Otherwise, you are also at risk of survivorship bias.
Survivorship bias is a common logical error, but you can take several steps to avoid it:
Being aware of survivorship bias, as well as being open and transparent about your assumptions, is the best strategy to prevent it.
Common types of selection bias are:
Bias affects the validity and reliability of your findings, leading to false conclusions and a misinterpretation of the truth. This can have serious implications in areas like medical research where new forms of treatment are being evaluated.
Reliability and validity are both about how well a method measures something:
If you are doing experimental research, you also have to consider the internal and external validity of your experiment.
The post What Is Survivorship Bias? | Definition & Examples appeared first on Scribbr.
]]>